Thursday, August 27, 2020

DÉJÀ VU IN HINDSIGHT - Why we believe in false premonitions - Researchers already know that déjà vu — the feeling that we have already had a particular experience before and are now reliving it — can come with a false sense of premonition. But is it also linked with a sense of postdiction — the feeling that our false premonition was, in fact, correct? Have you ever turned a corner into a street that you had never been on before and had a nagging feeling that some time, you had turned that same corner into that same street? If so, you have experienced what is known as “déjà vu.” If, following such an experience, you have also thought that you knew what would come next — say, that a black cat was about to cross your path in a hurry — then you have experienced a false premonition, which is often associated with déjà vu. Déjà vu and its associated phenomena have interested cognitive scientist Anne Cleary. Cleary and a fellow researcher, focused on the sense of false premonition that tends to accompany déjà vu and concluded that this likely happens because of the programming of our brains. Humans amass and store memories for predictive purposes — when we face a situation, we access previous similar experiences so that we can predict the likely outcomes automatically and thus make the best choices. With a phenomenon such as déjà vu, our brains become “tricked” into thinking that they can rely on previous experience to predict what will come up next.

...................................................................................................................................................

Déjà vu in hindsight

Why we believe in false premonitions

Written by Maria Cohut, Ph.D.

Fact checked by Isabel Godfrey


 

Researchers already know that déjà vu — the feeling that we have already had a particular experience before and are now reliving it — can come with a false sense of premonition.

But is it also linked with a sense of postdiction — the feeling that our false premonition was, in fact, correct?

New research shows how déjà vu influences aspects of our recollection.

Have you ever turned a corner into a street that you had never been on before and had a nagging feeling that some time, perhaps in a different life, you had turned that same corner into that same street?

If so, you have experienced what is known as “déjà vu.”

If, following such an experience, you have also thought that you knew what would come next — say, that a black cat was about to cross your path in a hurry — then you have experienced a false premonition, which is often associated with déjà vu.

Déjà vu and its associated phenomena have interested cognitive scientist Anne Cleary, from Colorado State University in Fort Collins, for many years.

In a previous study that Medical News Today covered, Cleary and a fellow researcher, Alexander Claxton, focused on the sense of false premonition that tends to accompany déjà vu and concluded that this likely happens because of the programming of our brains.

Humans, Cleary and Claxton explained, amass and store memories for predictive purposes — when we face a situation, we access previous similar experiences so that we can predict the likely outcomes automatically and thus make the best choices.

With a phenomenon such as déjà vu, our brains become “tricked” into thinking that they can rely on previous experience to predict what will come up next.

However, this is merely a false impression.

Now, Cleary and colleagues from Colorado State University are reporting their findings regarding another phenomenon relating to déjà vu: postdiction.

False familiarity tricks the brain

When a person experiences postdiction, they are “filling in” memory gaps with newer information, but they remain under the mistaken impression that this information was already part of the original memory.

So far, it has been unclear whether déjà vu has as strong an association with postdiction as it does with false premonitions.

However, in the new study paper that they published in the Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, Cleary and colleagues now report that it is and explain why this may be so.

“If this is an illusion — just a feeling — why do people so strongly believe they actually predicted what unfolded next? I wondered if there was an explanation in some sort of cognitive illusion,” says Cleary.

To find out, the researchers tasked study participants with the exploration of a virtual reality scene and then asked them whether they were experiencing déjà vu.

After this, the participants returned to the virtual scene, which randomly turned to the left or right.

At this point, the researchers asked the participants whether the event had unfolded as they had expected.

Finally, in another experiment, a second cohort of participants went through the same series of actions, with the additional task of rating how familiar the scene was to them both before and after the turn took place.

The researchers found that when participants experienced déjà vu and also reported a strong sense that they could predict what would happen next, this situation was strongly associated with the phenomenon of postdiction.

These participants were convinced, in hindsight, that they had correctly predicted the direction of the turn in the scene.

However, as the turns occurred at random, the researchers explain, they would have been pretty much impossible to foresee.

Cleary and colleagues argue that this mistaken belief in the accuracy of a false prediction is likely to be due to the intense sense of familiarity that the déjà vu sensation provides.

“If the entire scene feels familiar as it unfolds, that might trick our brains into thinking we got it right after all. Because it felt so familiar as you were going through it, it felt like you knew all along how it was going to go, even if that could not have been the case.” Anne Cleary

In the future, Cleary is planning to put her findings to good use in a clinical context.

She says that she will be joining forces with neuroscientists from Emory University in Atlanta, GA, to conduct a study focusing on individuals who have injuries of the medial temporal lobe of the brain.

The researcher explains that people with such injuries often experience seizures that recurring experiences of déjà vu accompany.

The forthcoming study may offer a glimpse into the underlying biological mechanisms at play in this phenomenon.

Maria Cohut, Ph.D.

Maria is an insatiably curious soul, particularly fascinated by the mysterious workings of the human brain, medical history, and our relationship with our own bodies, both during and after life. Before joining Medical News Today, Maria worked as a teacher, academic ambassador, and a freelance writer and copy editor. Recently, she finished a Ph.D. in English at the University of Warwick in the U.K. In her spare time, she learns Japanese, occasionally practices taxidermy, and spreads her infectious love of invertebrates.

Medical News Today, part of Healthline Media UK, is one of the fastest growing health information sites in the United States. Every month, more than 70 million people come to Medical News Today for our in-depth health content and the latest news in medical research. Consider us your intellectual peer. We unravel the complexities of health information and make it easy to understand.

We take a deep dive into medical research of the past and present, and we break it down to give you the clear-cut facts. Ultimately, we want you to be confident in making informed health decisions for yourself and your loved ones. Using peer reviewed studies and reputable sources, our knowledgeable and curious editors and writers create more than 250 pieces of content every month.

https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/326953.php#1


No comments:

Post a Comment